Advertisement
Regular Research Article|Articles in Press

Sex Differences in the Relationship between Perceived Stress and Cognitive Trajectories

Open AccessPublished:November 23, 2022DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jagp.2022.11.009

      Highlights

      • What is the primary question addressed by this study?
        We examined sex differences in stress-related cognitive decline and peripheral inflammatory biomarker trajectories over time in older adults.
      • What is the main finding of this study?
        We found that higher levels of perceived stress related to steeper cognitive declines and accelerated increases in peripheral inflammatory biomarkers over time in older men only. Longitudinal associations between perceived stress, cognition, and inflammation were significantly attenuated in older women.
      • What is the meaning of the finding?
        Our results suggest that older men may be particularly vulnerable to negative effects of stress (a modifiable risk factor) on cognitive decline.

      ABSTRACT

      Objective

      Chronic stress adversely affects cognition, in part due to stress-induced inflammation. Rodent models suggest females are more resilient against stress-related cognitive dysfunction than males; however, few studies have examined this in humans. We examined sex differences in the relationship between perceived stress, cognitive functioning, and peripheral inflammation over time among cognitively normal older adults.

      Design

      Longitudinal observational study.

      Setting

      University research center.

      Participants

      274 community-dwelling older adults (baseline age: M=70.7, SD=7.2; 58% women; Clinical Dementia Rating=0) who completed at least two study visits.

      Measurements

      Neurocognitive functioning and perceived stress (Perceived Stress Scale [PSS]) were assessed at each visit. Plasma was analyzed for interleukin 6 (IL-6) and tumor necrosis factor alpha (TNF-α) in a subset of 147 participants. Linear mixed effects models examined the interaction between average PSS (i.e., averaged within persons across visits), sex, and time on cognitive domains and on inflammatory markers.

      Results

      The interaction between stress, sex, and time predicted executive functioning (β = 0.26, SE = 0.10, p = 0.01) such that higher average PSS related to steeper declines in men, but not in women. Among the 147 participants with inflammatory data, higher average PSS was associated with steeper increases in IL-6 over time in men, but not in women.

      Conclusion

      Consistent with animal models, results showed older men were more vulnerable to negative effects of stress on cognitive aging, with domain-specific declines in executive function. Findings also suggest systemic immunological mechanisms may underlie increased risk for cognitive decline in men with higher levels of stress. Future work is needed to examine the potential efficacy of person-specific stress interventions.

      Keywords

      INTRODUCTION

      The prevalence of dementia is increasing worldwide and will continue to grow exponentially in the coming decades.
      • Cao Q
      • Tan CC
      • Xu W
      • et al.
      The prevalence of dementia: a systematic review and meta-analysis.
      The negative impacts of dementia are high-stakes and far-reaching, including worse quality of life on the individual and family-systems level
      • Farina N
      • Page TE
      • Daley S
      • et al.
      Factors associated with the quality of life of family carers of people with dementia: a systematic review.
      as well as on the societal and economic level with dementia care costs growing at a faster rate than dementia prevalence itself.
      • Xu J
      • Qiu C
      Worldwide economic costs and societal burden of dementia.
      Given the lack of effective pharmacological treatments for all-cause dementia, there is a need to identify modifiable behavioral risk factors using a precision medicine approach.
      • Yaffe K
      Modifiable risk factors and prevention of dementia: what is the latest evidence?.
      ,
      • Isaacson RS
      • Ganzer CA
      • Hristov H
      • et al.
      The clinical practice of risk reduction for Alzheimer's disease: a precision medicine approach.
      Cardiovascular risk factors (e.g., hypertension, hyperlipidemia), cognitive training, and physical activity show promise as modifiable risk factors for dementia.

      National Academies of Sciences E, Medicine: preventing cognitive decline and dementia: a way forward. 2017

      Nevertheless, the efficacy of dementia prevention interventions targeting these health domains is highly variable, necessitating examination of a wider range of modifiable risk factors (e.g., mental health, social activity)
      • Livingston G
      • Sommerlad A
      • Orgeta V
      • et al.
      Dementia prevention, intervention, and care.
      and the person-specific factors that may influence vulnerability to the detrimental effects of specific risks.
      Psychosocial stress is one such modifiable risk factor that has been consistently associated with worse levels of cognitive functioning, faster cognitive decline, and reduced brain volume in older adults.
      • Johansson L
      • Guo X
      • Waern M
      • et al.
      Midlife psychological stress and risk of dementia: a 35-year longitudinal population study.
      ,
      • Lupien SJ
      • McEwen BS
      • Gunnar MR
      • et al.
      Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on the brain, behavior and cognition.
      The relationship between psychosocial stress and cognitive and brain health are proposed to occur through several possible  mechanisms. The physiological response to chronic stress in animals and humans includes activation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis and subsequent secretion of glucocorticoids, which are neurotoxic when expressed at high concentrations for prolonged durations in brain regions with high density of glucocorticoid receptors (e.g., hippocampus, prefrontal cortex).
      • Lupien SJ
      • Evans A
      • Lord C
      • et al.
      Hippocampal volume is as variable in young as in older adults: implications for the notion of hippocampal atrophy in humans.
      ,
      • Lupien SJ
      • Juster RP
      • Raymond C
      • et al.
      The effects of chronic stress on the human brain: From neurotoxicity, to vulnerability, to opportunity.
      More recent work has also highlighted an important role of systemic low-grade chronic inflammation in the context of aging (i.e., “inflammaging”)
      • Franceschi C
      • Campisi J
      Chronic inflammation (inflammaging) and its potential contribution to age-associated diseases.
      and psychosocial stress.
      • Liu YZ
      • Wang YX
      • Jiang CL
      Inflammation: the common pathway of stress-related diseases.
      ,
      • Rohleder N
      Stimulation of systemic low-grade inflammation by psychosocial stress.
      Chronic inflammation is a robust biological mechanism underlying the development of cardiovascular conditions and dementia, which includes the accumulation of Alzheimer's disease proteins through microglia activation.
      • Bisht K
      • Sharma K
      • Tremblay ME
      Chronic stress as a risk factor for Alzheimer's disease: roles of microglia-mediated synaptic remodeling, inflammation, and oxidative stress.
      ,
      • Steptoe A
      • Kivimaki M
      Stress and cardiovascular disease.
      In fact, recent work by our group demonstrated that elevations in peripheral macrophage-related cytokines mediated the longitudinal relationship between age and executive functioning in clinically normal older adults with high levels of perceived stress.
      • Casaletto KB
      • Staffaroni AM
      • Elahi F
      • et al.
      Perceived stress is associated with accelerated monocyte/macrophage aging trajectories in clinically normal adults.
      Although findings linking stress to cognitive dysfunction are consistent on average, there is significant heterogeneity in the human stress response.
      • Bale TL
      • Epperson CN
      Sex differences and stress across the lifespan.
      ,
      • Kudielka BM
      • Kirschbaum C
      Sex differences in HPA axis responses to stress: a review.
      and few studies have examined person-specific factors that may alter individual vulnerability to the negative effects of stress on cognitive functioning.
      One important factor to consider as a moderator of the relationship between stress and cognition is sex/gender (hereafter referred to as “sex”). Recent research has highlighted significant sex differences in the onset, trajectory, and risk of cognitive decline in older adults.
      • Mazure CM
      • Swendsen J
      Sex differences in Alzheimer's disease and other dementias.
      • Mielke MM
      Sex and gender differences in Alzheimer's disease dementia.
      • Subramaniapillai S
      • Almey A
      • Rajah MN
      • et al.
      Sex and gender differences in cognitive and brain reserve: implications for Alzheimer's disease in women.
      Observed sex differences in dementia are complex and vary by neuropathological etiology, but in brief, women tend to have later onset of cognitive impairment with faster rate of decline.
      • Levine DA
      • Gross AL
      • Briceno EM
      • et al.
      Sex differences in cognitive decline among US adults.
      Risk factors also vary by sex, with the majority of studies focusing on sex-dependent cardiovascular risk for dementia.
      • Gannon OJ
      • Robison LS
      • Custozzo AJ
      • et al.
      Sex differences in risk factors for vascular contributions to cognitive impairment & dementia.
      Importantly, sex also influences physiological and behavioral responses to stress. Reproductive hormones play an important role in shaping neural stress-related circuitry beginning in utero (e.g., density and distribution of glucocorticoid receptors in the brain) and mediating HPA axis function throughout adulthood and older adulthood, particularly during reproductive senescence.
      • Bale TL
      • Epperson CN
      Sex differences and stress across the lifespan.
      Many studies examining sex differences in stress response in older adults have focused on changes that women undergo pre- and postmenopause, with findings demonstrating that post-menopausal women have an increased cortisol response to stress compared to age-matched men.
      • Kudielka BM
      • Kirschbaum C
      Sex differences in HPA axis responses to stress: a review.
      Consistent evidence in rodent models, however, have shown that the cognitive consequences of chronic stress tend to be worse in males compared to females.
      • Luine V
      • Gomez J
      • Beck K
      • et al.
      Sex differences in chronic stress effects on cognition in rodents.
      These animal studies suggest that estrogens contribute to cognitive resilience in the context of chronic stress and benefits persist even when estrogens are no longer produced peripherally.
      • McLaughlin KJ
      • Baran SE
      • Wright RL
      • et al.
      Chronic stress enhances spatial memory in ovariectomized female rats despite CA3 dendritic retraction: possible involvement of CA1 neurons.
      Despite clear clinical implications of this research for person-specific dementia prevention, to our knowledge, no studies to date have examined sex differences in stress-related cognition in humans.
      The aims of this study were to: 1) examine whether the longitudinal relationship between perceived stress and cognitive decline in older adults differs by sex; and 2) examine peripheral inflammation as a mechanism underlying sex-dependent relationships between perceived stress and cognitive  decline. We hypothesized that men would exhibit a stronger relationship between perceived stress and cognitive decline than women. We also hypothesized that higher perceived stress would relate to increases in inflammation  over time and that this relationship would be stronger in men than women.

      METHODS

      Participants

      Participants were 274 community-dwelling, cognitively normal older adults enrolled in the UCSF Memory and Aging Center's (MAC) Longitudinal Brain Aging Study. Inclusion criteria for the parent study include having no history or current evidence of the following: 1) a diagnosed neurological condition (e.g., epilepsy, large vessel stroke), 2) major medical (e.g., active neoplasm, HIV, dialysis), psychiatric (e.g., schizophrenia), or active substance use disorders, and 3) cognitive or functional decline as indicated by a Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR) of 0 via study partner interviews. The parent study includes a total of 408 older adults (56% female; mean age = 76.5 years; mean education = 17.4 years; 83% non-Hispanic White). Participants were only included in the current study if they had at least two complete parent study visits with neurocognitive and self-report psychological data. Data were collected from 2001 to 2020. All study procedures were approved by the UCSF Committee on Human Research. All participants provided written informed consent.

      Cognitive Testing Measures

      At each visit, participants completed a brief battery of neuropsychological tests assessing three domains: executive functioning, memory, and processing speed. Raw test scores were converted to baseline-sample-based z-scores and then averaged within each domain to create domain-specific composite z-scores. Executive functioning was assessed via a modified version of the Trail Making Test requiring participants to serially alternate between numbers and days of the week (total time to complete), a Stroop interference task (number of correct items in 60 seconds), design fluency (D-KEFS Condition 1), phonemic fluency (number of D words in 60 seconds),
      • Biesbroek JM
      • Lim J-S
      • Weaver NA
      • et al.
      Anatomy of phonemic and semantic fluency: a lesion and disconnectome study in 1231 stroke patients.
      ,
      • Schmidt CS
      • Nitschke K
      • Bormann T
      • et al.
      Dissociating frontal and temporal correlates of phonological and semantic fluency in a large sample of left hemisphere stroke patients.
      and digit span backward (longest span).
      • Kramer JH
      • Jurik J
      • Sha SJ
      • et al.
      Distinctive neuropsychological patterns in frontotemporal dementia, semantic dementia, and Alzheimer disease.
      ,
      • Staffaroni AM
      • Asken BM
      • Casaletto KB
      • et al.
      Development and validation of the Uniform Data Set (v3. 0) executive function composite score (UDS3-EF).
      Memory was assessed via the California Verbal Learning Test-Second Edition (CVLT-II; total immediate recall, long delay free recall, and recognition discriminability) and Benson Figure Recall.
      • Kramer JH
      • Jurik J
      • Sha SJ
      • et al.
      Distinctive neuropsychological patterns in frontotemporal dementia, semantic dementia, and Alzheimer disease.
      Processing speed was assessed via computerized visuospatial processing speed tasks described previously.
      • Kerchner GA
      • Racine CA
      • Hale S
      • et al.
      Cognitive processing speed in older adults: relationship with white matter integrity.
      Higher memory and executive functioning z-scores reflect better performance, whereas lower processing speed scores indicate better performance (i.e., faster reaction time).

      Self-Reported Psychological Measures

      Participants completed self-report measures of stress and mood at each visit. The Perceived Stress Scale (PSS) is a well-validated, 10-item self-report measure that assesses the degree to which individuals perceive recent life events/situations as stressful.
      • Cohen S
      • Kamarck T
      • Mermelstein R
      A global measure of perceived stress.
      Response options to each item are on a Likert-type scale from 0 (never) to 4 (very often). Responses to each item are summed to create a total score (ranging from 0–40), resulting in higher scores reflecting higher levels of perceived stress. Established ranges of clinical severity include low stress (0–13), moderate stress (14–26), and high stress (27–40). To account for depressive symptoms in analyses, the 30-item Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS) was used.
      • Yesavage JA
      Geriatric depression scale.
      To best capture participants’ long-term levels of perceived stress and depressive symptoms over time, scores for each measure were averaged over all visits within persons, resulting in one mean PSS score and one mean GDS score for each participant.

      Inflammatory Biomarkers

      A subset of 147 participants completed a fasting blood draw during at least two study visits. Blood was centrifuged at 2000 x g for 15 minutes at 4°C to collect plasma, which was then transferred to 500 µL polypropylene cryovials for long-term storage at -80°C. Prior to assay initiation, samples were gradually brought back up to room temperature. Interleukin 6 (IL-6) and tumor necrosis factor alpha (TNF-α) were selected as biomarkers of interest because they are well-studied proinflammatory cytokines that are robustly involved in the human inflammatory stress response.
      • Kiecolt-Glaser JK
      • Preacher KJ
      • MacCallum RC
      • et al.
      Chronic stress and age-related increases in the proinflammatory cytokine IL-6.
      ,
      • Marsland AL
      • Walsh C
      • Lockwood K
      • et al.
      The effects of acute psychological stress on circulating and stimulated inflammatory markers: a systematic review and meta-analysis.
      IL-6 and TNF-α were obtained using the human proinflammatory panel 1 V-PLEX kit provided by Meso Scale Diagnostics, LLC (Rockville, MD). Values were log-transformed to achieve normality.

      Statistical Analyses

      To compare baseline demographic and clinical characteristics by sex, one-way ANOVA or chi-squared tests were used for continuous or categorical variables, respectively. Next, linear mixed effects models were used to examine the interaction between mean PSS score, sex, and time on each cognitive domain outcome, covarying for mean age, education, and mean depressive symptoms. Mean depressive symptoms were included in the model to ensure that effects of perceived stress were independent of depressive symptomology. Person-specific random intercepts and a random effect of time were specified. Including these random effects allows the model to estimate individual intercepts (i.e., levels of the outcome at time = 0) and individual slopes (i.e., trajectories of the outcome over time for each person), which more accurately models whether the time-invariant between-person factors (i.e., sex and average perceived stress) explain variance in individual time slopes. Non-significant three-way interactions were removed from the model to examine the interaction between mean PSS score and time in the entire cohort (i.e., regardless of sex). To examine the second study aim, linear mixed effects models were used to examine the interaction between mean PSS score, sex, and time on each inflammatory biomarker (i.e., IL-6 and TNF-α), covarying for mean age, education, and mean depressive symptoms. Person-specific random intercepts and a random effect of time were specified. Estimates from all regression models are reported as standardized betas (β), which describe the strength of the relationship between predictor and outcome in units of standard deviations (i.e., a 1 standard deviation increase in the predictor corresponds to a β standard deviation change in the outcome). All analyses were conducted in R, version 4.0.5.

      RESULTS

      Participant demographic and clinical characteristics by sex are displayed in Table 1. On average, participants were about 70 years old (range = 52–91), 58% female, had greater than a college education (mean = 17.5 years), were mostly non-Hispanic White (88%), and were longitudinally followed for about 7 years (mean total study visits  = 5.56, range  = 2-15). There were no differences in length of study follow up by sex. Women had significantly less education than men and reported significantly higher PSS scores on average, but depressive symptoms were similar by sex (see Table 1).
      TABLE 1Participant Demographic and Clinical Characteristics by Sex (N=274).
      Men (n=116)Women (n=158)Test Statistic (df)p-Value
      Demographics
       Baseline Age71 (7.2)70 (7.2)t = 1.2 (248)0.229
       Education (yrs)18.1 (1.9)17.0 (2.1)t = 4.4 (263)<0.001
       Race/Ethnicityχ2 = 10.2 (7)0.177
        Non-Hispanic White102 (88%)140 (89%)
        Asian/Pacific Islander11 (9%)7 (4%)
        Black0 (0%)2 (1%)
        Other/not Specified3 (3%)9 (6%)
      Time in Study
       Total Study Visits5.66 (3.06)5.48 (3.20)t = 0.5 (254)0.635
       Total Years of Follow Up7.01 (3.99)6.69 (4.00)t = 0.7 (248)0.508
      Cognitive Z Scores
       Baseline executive function0.70 (0.61)0.90 (0.62)t = -0.3 (258)0.754
       Baseline memory-0.08 (0.74)0.16 (0.69)t = -2.7 (233)0.008
       Baseline processing speed-0.06 (0.97)0.04 (1.02)t = -0.6 (190)0.518
      Psychological
       Mean PSS (possible range=0-40)8.84 (4.95)10.83 (5.13)t = -3.2 (253)0.001
       Mean PSS Severity Levelχ2 = 4.2 (2)0.124
        Low Stress (0-13)96 (83%)115 (73%)
        Moderate stress (13–26)20 (17%)42 (26%)
        High stress (>26)0 (0%)1 (1%)
      Mean GDS (possible range=0-30)2.71 (2.83)2.73 (2.76)t = -0.1 (244)0.959
      Abbreviations:. PSS , perceived stress scale; GDS , geriatric depression scale.
      Linear mixed effects models revealed a significant interaction between mean PSS, sex, and time on executive functioning (β = 0.261, SE = 0.103, p = 0.012; Table 2). Follow-up analyses stratified by sex showed that higher mean PSS score was associated with steeper declines in executive functioning over time in men (β  = -0.274, SE  = 0.055, p  <  0.001); however, this relationship was significantly attenuated in women (β  = -0.013, SE  = 0.083, p = 0.877; Fig. 1A). The three-way interaction between mean PSS, sex, and time was not a significant predictor for memory (β  = 0.004, SE  = 0.106, p  =  0.973; Fig. 1B) nor processing speed (β  = -0.115, SE  = 0.125, p  =  0.359; Fig. 1C). After removing the non-significant three-way interaction, there was a significant relationship between higher mean PSS and steeper declines in memory (β  = -0.132, SE  = 0.055, p  =  0.018); however, the association between higher mean PSS and processing speed decline was not statistically significant (β  =  0.091, SE  =  0.064, p =  0.158).
      TABLE 2Results of Linear Mixed Effects Models Examining Differential Relationships Between Perceived Stress and Cognitive Trajectories by Sex
      Cognitive Domain Outcome
      Executive FunctioningMemoryProcessing Speed
      Mean Age-0.296 (0.045)
      p<0.001.
      -0.222 (0.047)
      p<0.001.
      0.261 (0.051)
      p<0.001.
      Education0.106 (0.050)
      p<0.05;
      0.135 (0.050)
      p<0.01;
      -0.017 (0.054)
      Mean GDS0.054 (0.056)0.032 (0.057)0.056 (0.061)
      Mean PSS-0.067 (0.086)-0.016 (0.090)0.136 (0.094)
      Sex (ref: men)0.022 (0.112)0.248 (0.118)
      p<0.05;
      0.034 (0.122)
      Time0.284 (0.070)
      p<0.001.
      0.060 (0.071)0.066 (0.083)
      Mean PSS × Sex-0.003 (0.132)-0.052 (0.140)-0.027 (0.146)
      Mean PSS × Time-0.297 (0.085)
      p<0.001.
      -0.136 (0.086)0.173 (0.101)
      Sex × Time-0.217 (0.090)
      p<0.05;
      0.006 (0.092)0.040 (0.110)
      Mean PSS × Sex × Time0.261 (0.103)
      p<0.05;
      0.004 (0.106)-0.115 (0.125)
      Note. Values are standardized regression estimates (SE)
      a p<0.05;
      b p<0.01;
      c p<0.001.
      FIGURE 1
      FIGURE 1Three-way interaction between mean perceived stress score (PSS), sex, and time on each cognitive outcome: (A) Executive functioning; (B) Memory; (C) Processing speed (y-axis reversed to show higher scores/worse performance at the bottom).
      Among the subset of 147 UCSF MAC participants who had at least two timepoints with inflammatory biomarker data, linear mixed effects models revealed a significant interaction between mean PSS, sex, and time on IL-6 (β  = -0.378, SE  = 0.185, p  =  0.043; Fig. 2A), but not on TNF-α (β  = -0.138, SE  = 0.153, p  =  0.370; Fig. 2B). Follow-up analyses stratified by sex showed that higher mean PSS score was associated with greater increases in IL-6 over time in men (β  = 0.246, SE  =  0.119, p  =  0.043); however, this relationship was not significant in women (β  = -0.007, SE  =  0.120, p  =  0.955). All models examining relationships among stress, sex, and time on cognitive and inflammatory outcomes were repeated without covarying for mean depressive symptoms, and all results held.
      FIGURE 2
      FIGURE 2Three-way interaction between mean perceived stress score (PSS), sex, and time on each inflammatory biomarker: (A) IL-6 and (B) TNF-α.

      DISCUSSION

      Gaining a deeper understanding of person-specific vulnerability or resilience to different behavioral risk factors for cognitive impairment is imperative for dementia prevention in the rapidly growing older adult population. Our findings showed that among older adults without cognitive impairment, men were more vulnerable to the negative effects of perceived stress on cognitive decline than women, and that this may be related to corresponding increases in inflammation over time. Notably, these associations were significant even in the context of a sample with low-to-moderate levels of stress on average. Although cognitive scores across all visits were within the range of normal performance, within-person declines in this range are still of clinical significance as such declines are often associated with subjective cognitive changes and precede conversion to mild cognitive impairment.
      • Koppara A
      • Wagner M
      • Lange C
      • et al.
      Cognitive performance before and after the onset of subjective cognitive decline in old age.
      ,
      • Papp KV
      • Buckley R
      • Mormino E
      • et al.
      Clinical meaningfulness of subtle cognitive decline on longitudinal testing in preclinical AD.
      The observed interactions between perceived stress and sex on cognitive decline are consistent with our hypotheses and previous animal models demonstrating that males under chronic stress have worse neurobehavioral outcomes than females.
      • Bale TL
      • Epperson CN
      Sex differences and stress across the lifespan.
      ,
      • Luine V
      • Gomez J
      • Beck K
      • et al.
      Sex differences in chronic stress effects on cognition in rodents.
      Although the broad negative impact of stress on cognitive aging has been well-studied, this is the first study to our knowledge to show sex-dependent relationships between stress and cognitive decline in humans. These associations were also independent of depressed mood. Overall, the observed sex-dependent relationships among stress, inflammation, and cognitive functioning highlight the need to consider individual differences when examining modifiable risk factors for cognitive decline.
      The combination of results, including sex-dependent relationships specific to executive functioning in addition to the association between stress and inflammation, suggest that our findings may be driven by immuno- and cerebrovascular mechanisms. First, the observed relationship between higher perceived stress and increases in IL-6 among men suggest that the sex-dependent relationship between perceived stress and cognitive decline may be related to increases in peripheral inflammation. This longitudinal relationship between perceived stress and IL-6 is consistent with robust findings linking increases in IL-6 to chronic stress.
      • Kiecolt-Glaser JK
      • Preacher KJ
      • MacCallum RC
      • et al.
      Chronic stress and age-related increases in the proinflammatory cytokine IL-6.
      Direct pathways between peripheral inflammatory biomarkers and cognitive functioning are still not well understood, as peripheral levels of cytokines are highly pleiotropic and only variably relate to central nervous system inflammatory markers.
      • Bettcher BM
      • Johnson SC
      • Fitch R
      • et al.
      Cerebrospinal fluid and plasma levels of inflammation differentially relate to CNS markers of Alzheimer's disease pathology and neuronal damage.
      It is possible that the observed plasma IL-6 levels reflect systemic immunovascular changes with age and disease.
      • Michaud M
      • Balardy L
      • Moulis G
      • et al.
      Proinflammatory cytokines, aging, and age-related diseases.
      ,
      • Villar-Fincheira P
      • Sanhueza-Olivares F
      • Norambuena-Soto I
      • et al.
      Role of interleukin-6 in vascular health and disease.
      Higher levels of plasma cytokines are associated with increased dementia risk and are closely related to cerebrovascular integrity.
      • Zuliani G
      • Ranzini M
      • Guerra G
      • et al.
      Plasma cytokines profile in older subjects with late onset Alzheimer's disease or vascular dementia.
      Men are also at a higher risk of cardio- and cerebrovascular disease compared to age-matched women.
      • Leening MJ
      • Ferket BS
      • Steyerberg EW
      • et al.
      Sex differences in lifetime risk and first manifestation of cardiovascular disease: prospective population based cohort study.
      Thus, our findings may reflect the adverse immunovascular impact of chronic stress, even when relatively low in severity, in individuals at highest risk of vascular disease (i.e., men) with subsequent indirect effects on the brain (i.e., chronic inflammation may increase vulnerability to cognitive decline through the development of cardiovascular conditions known to affect brain health).
      • Fornage M
      • Chiang YA
      • O'Meara ES
      • et al.
      Biomarkers of inflammation and MRI-Defined small vessel disease of the brain: the Cardiovascular Health Study.
      This represents an important mechanism underlying stress, as levels of IL-6 in plasma are feasible to monitor in a clinical setting.
      In addition, the differential effect of stress on cognition by sex was observed only for the domain of executive functioning. Neurobiological underpinnings of executive functioning include both frontal cortical regions and broader white matter networks (e.g., fronto-parietal),
      • Bettcher BM
      • Mungas D
      • Patel N
      • et al.
      Neuroanatomical substrates of executive functions: beyond prefrontal structures.
      some of which are particularly sensitive to chronic cerebrovascular changes.
      • Kramer J
      • Reed BR
      • Mungas D
      • et al.
      Executive dysfunction in subcortical ischaemic vascular disease.
      Thus, this finding is also consistent with our hypothesis that the observed relationships among sex, stress, inflammation, and cognition may be driven by immuno- and cerebrovascular mechanisms. In contrast, higher stress was associated with steeper memory declines in both men and women equally. Other mechanisms may play a role in the negative effects of stress on memory, such as promotion of underlying neurodegenerative pathology.
      • Bisht K
      • Sharma K
      • Tremblay ME
      Chronic stress as a risk factor for Alzheimer's disease: roles of microglia-mediated synaptic remodeling, inflammation, and oxidative stress.
      Further work is needed to elucidate unique mechanisms of stress on cognition that are both sex-dependent and sex-independent to inform person-specific treatments and precision dementia prevention.
      Additional mechanisms underlying the observed sex-dependent relationships likely include sex hormones. In animal models, female resilience to stress on cognition is hypothesized to be related to estrogens even in ovariectomized rodents,
      • McLaughlin KJ
      • Baran SE
      • Wright RL
      • et al.
      Chronic stress enhances spatial memory in ovariectomized female rats despite CA3 dendritic retraction: possible involvement of CA1 neurons.
      partly due to the neurodevelopmental effects of estrogen on hippocampal morphology
      • Spencer JL
      • Waters EM
      • Romeo RD
      • et al.
      Uncovering the mechanisms of estrogen effects on hippocampal function.
      and the continued local production of estradiol in the hippocampus, hypothalamus, and prefrontal cortex following ovariectomy or reproductive senescence.
      • Kato A
      • Hojo Y
      • Higo S
      • et al.
      Female hippocampal estrogens have a significant correlation with cyclic fluctuation of hippocampal spines.
      ,
      • Cui J
      • Shen Y
      • Li R
      Estrogen synthesis and signaling pathways during aging: from periphery to brain.
      In addition, estrogens and estrogen receptors play an important role in cardiovascular protection throughout the lifespan
      • Aryan L
      • Younessi D
      • Zargari M
      • et al.
      The role of estrogen receptors in cardiovascular disease.
      and longitudinal studies have shown that higher estradiol levels are associated with reduced cardiovascular risk in postmenopausal women.
      • Zhao D
      • Guallar E
      • Ouyang P
      • et al.
      Endogenous sex hormones and incident cardiovascular disease in post-menopausal women.
      This translational work supports our sex-dependent findings in older adults, including postmenopausal women; however, future work is still needed to better understand the interplay of sex hormones, chromosomes, and cognitive risk and resilience in humans.
      This study has several strengths, including the utilization of longitudinal data and examination of a posited biological mechanism underlying the behavioral relationships of interest; however, it is not without limitations. First, our data did not include any objective measures of stress (e.g., cortisol). Although our intention was to examine the subjective perception of psychosocial stress on cognition, it may be beneficial for future studies to incorporate cortisol levels to better understand its contribution to sex differences in the relationship between perceived stress and cognitive functioning. Next, our sample included only individuals who were known to be cognitively normal at each visit, which potentially introduces survival bias and limits the generalizability of these findings to those with known neurodegenerative diseases (e.g., Alzheimer's disease, Lewy body disease) and/or who eventually convert to having mild cognitive impairment and dementia. Future work is needed to better understand how lowering stress and inflammation in those with high risk for dementia (e.g., older adults with neurodegenerative pathology) affects cognitive trajectories, particularly among men. Additionally, we did not have additional data on participants’ engagement in stress-modifying activities (e.g., yoga, mindfulness) or experience of major life events that impact stress (e.g., moving, new medical diagnoses, death of loved ones). Future work is need to examine whether there are acute, temporal relationships between environmental stressors, perceived stress, and changes in cognitive functioning, and how these may be influenced by stress-modifying activities.
      Overall, our findings suggest that older men with higher levels of stress may be at risk for cognitive decline. Future studies replicating sex-dependent effects of stress on cognition in older adults may inform development of person-specific intervention strategies. Additional work is also needed to examine whether interventions to monitor and reduce stress in older adults, particularly among men, influence trajectories of systemic inflammation and cognitive performance. Finally, future research that identifies modifiable risk factors using a person-specific framework for cognitive decline is urgently needed to inform clinical practice for precision dementia prevention.

      AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS

      Emily W. Paolillo, Ph.D. contributed to study design, data analysis, interpretation, and manuscript writing and revision.
      Michelle You, B.A. contributed to data collection, data management, and revision of manuscript for intellectual content.
      Eva Gontrum, B.A. contributed to data collection, data management, and revision of manuscript for intellectual content.
      Rowan Saloner, Ph.D. revised the manuscript for intellectual content.
      Leslie S. Gaynor, Ph.D. revised the manuscript for intellectual content.
      Joel H. Kramer, Psy.D. contributed to study design, conceptualization, and revision of manuscript intellectual content.
      Kaitlin B. Casaletto, Ph.D. contributed to study design, conceptualization, data interpretation, and revision of manuscript for intellectual content.

      Declaration of Competing Interest

      The authors have no  disclosures to report.

      Acknowledgments/Grant Support and Financial Disclosure

      This study was supported by NIH-NIA grants K23AG058752 (PI: KBC), R01AG072475 (PI: KBC), R01AG032289 (PI: JHK), R01AG048234 (PI: JHK), UCSF ADRC P30 AG062422 (PI: Rabinovici). Our work was also supported by Larry L. Hillblom Network Grant (2014-A-004-NET; PI: JHK) and the Alzheimer's Association (PI: KBC; AARG-20-683875).

      PREVIOUS PRESENTATION

      This work was presented during the 50th annual meeting of the International Neuropsychological Society (New Orleans, LA) in February 2022.

      REFERENCES

        • Cao Q
        • Tan CC
        • Xu W
        • et al.
        The prevalence of dementia: a systematic review and meta-analysis.
        J Alzheimers Dis. 2020; 73: 1157-1166
        • Farina N
        • Page TE
        • Daley S
        • et al.
        Factors associated with the quality of life of family carers of people with dementia: a systematic review.
        Alzheimers Dement. 2017; 13: 572-581
        • Xu J
        • Qiu C
        Worldwide economic costs and societal burden of dementia.
        Biomarkers for Preclinical Alzheimer's Disease. Springer, 2018: 3-13
        • Yaffe K
        Modifiable risk factors and prevention of dementia: what is the latest evidence?.
        JAMA Intern Med. 2018; 178: 281-282
        • Isaacson RS
        • Ganzer CA
        • Hristov H
        • et al.
        The clinical practice of risk reduction for Alzheimer's disease: a precision medicine approach.
        Alzheimers Dement. 2018; 14: 1663-1673
      1. National Academies of Sciences E, Medicine: preventing cognitive decline and dementia: a way forward. 2017

        • Livingston G
        • Sommerlad A
        • Orgeta V
        • et al.
        Dementia prevention, intervention, and care.
        Lancet. 2017; 390: 2673-2734
        • Johansson L
        • Guo X
        • Waern M
        • et al.
        Midlife psychological stress and risk of dementia: a 35-year longitudinal population study.
        Brain. 2010; 133: 2217-2224
        • Lupien SJ
        • McEwen BS
        • Gunnar MR
        • et al.
        Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on the brain, behavior and cognition.
        Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009; 10: 434-445
        • Lupien SJ
        • Evans A
        • Lord C
        • et al.
        Hippocampal volume is as variable in young as in older adults: implications for the notion of hippocampal atrophy in humans.
        Neuroimage. 2007; 34: 479-485
        • Lupien SJ
        • Juster RP
        • Raymond C
        • et al.
        The effects of chronic stress on the human brain: From neurotoxicity, to vulnerability, to opportunity.
        Front Neuroendocrinol. 2018; 49: 91-105
        • Franceschi C
        • Campisi J
        Chronic inflammation (inflammaging) and its potential contribution to age-associated diseases.
        J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2014; 69: S4-S9
        • Liu YZ
        • Wang YX
        • Jiang CL
        Inflammation: the common pathway of stress-related diseases.
        Front Hum Neurosci. 2017; 11: 316
        • Rohleder N
        Stimulation of systemic low-grade inflammation by psychosocial stress.
        Psychosom Med. 2014; 76: 181-189
        • Bisht K
        • Sharma K
        • Tremblay ME
        Chronic stress as a risk factor for Alzheimer's disease: roles of microglia-mediated synaptic remodeling, inflammation, and oxidative stress.
        Neurobiol Stress. 2018; 9: 9-21
        • Steptoe A
        • Kivimaki M
        Stress and cardiovascular disease.
        Nat Rev Cardiol. 2012; 9: 360-370
        • Casaletto KB
        • Staffaroni AM
        • Elahi F
        • et al.
        Perceived stress is associated with accelerated monocyte/macrophage aging trajectories in clinically normal adults.
        Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2018; 26: 952-963
        • Bale TL
        • Epperson CN
        Sex differences and stress across the lifespan.
        Nat Neurosci. 2015; 18: 1413-1420
        • Kudielka BM
        • Kirschbaum C
        Sex differences in HPA axis responses to stress: a review.
        Biol Psychol. 2005; 69: 113-132
        • Mazure CM
        • Swendsen J
        Sex differences in Alzheimer's disease and other dementias.
        Lancet Neurol. 2016; 15: 451-452
        • Mielke MM
        Sex and gender differences in Alzheimer's disease dementia.
        Psychiatr Times. 2018; 35: 14-17
        • Subramaniapillai S
        • Almey A
        • Rajah MN
        • et al.
        Sex and gender differences in cognitive and brain reserve: implications for Alzheimer's disease in women.
        Front Neuroendocrinol. 2021; 60100879
        • Levine DA
        • Gross AL
        • Briceno EM
        • et al.
        Sex differences in cognitive decline among US adults.
        JAMA Netw Open. 2021; 4e210169
        • Gannon OJ
        • Robison LS
        • Custozzo AJ
        • et al.
        Sex differences in risk factors for vascular contributions to cognitive impairment & dementia.
        Neurochem Int. 2019; 127: 38-55
        • Luine V
        • Gomez J
        • Beck K
        • et al.
        Sex differences in chronic stress effects on cognition in rodents.
        Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2017; 152: 13-19
        • McLaughlin KJ
        • Baran SE
        • Wright RL
        • et al.
        Chronic stress enhances spatial memory in ovariectomized female rats despite CA3 dendritic retraction: possible involvement of CA1 neurons.
        Neuroscience. 2005; 135: 1045-1054
        • Biesbroek JM
        • Lim J-S
        • Weaver NA
        • et al.
        Anatomy of phonemic and semantic fluency: a lesion and disconnectome study in 1231 stroke patients.
        cortex. 2021; 143: 148-163
        • Schmidt CS
        • Nitschke K
        • Bormann T
        • et al.
        Dissociating frontal and temporal correlates of phonological and semantic fluency in a large sample of left hemisphere stroke patients.
        Neuroimage: Clinical. 2019; 23101840
        • Kramer JH
        • Jurik J
        • Sha SJ
        • et al.
        Distinctive neuropsychological patterns in frontotemporal dementia, semantic dementia, and Alzheimer disease.
        Cogn Behav Neurol. 2003; 16: 211-218
        • Staffaroni AM
        • Asken BM
        • Casaletto KB
        • et al.
        Development and validation of the Uniform Data Set (v3. 0) executive function composite score (UDS3-EF).
        Alzheimer's & Dementia. 2021; 17: 574-583
        • Kerchner GA
        • Racine CA
        • Hale S
        • et al.
        Cognitive processing speed in older adults: relationship with white matter integrity.
        PLoS One. 2012; 7: e50425
        • Cohen S
        • Kamarck T
        • Mermelstein R
        A global measure of perceived stress.
        J Health Soc Behav. 1983; 24: 385-396
        • Yesavage JA
        Geriatric depression scale.
        Psychopharmacol Bull. 1988; 24: 709-711
        • Kiecolt-Glaser JK
        • Preacher KJ
        • MacCallum RC
        • et al.
        Chronic stress and age-related increases in the proinflammatory cytokine IL-6.
        Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2003; 100: 9090-9095
        • Marsland AL
        • Walsh C
        • Lockwood K
        • et al.
        The effects of acute psychological stress on circulating and stimulated inflammatory markers: a systematic review and meta-analysis.
        Brain, Behav Immun. 2017; 64: 208-219
        • Koppara A
        • Wagner M
        • Lange C
        • et al.
        Cognitive performance before and after the onset of subjective cognitive decline in old age.
        Alzheimer's Dementia: Diagnosis, Assessment Dis Monitoring. 2015; 1: 194-205
        • Papp KV
        • Buckley R
        • Mormino E
        • et al.
        Clinical meaningfulness of subtle cognitive decline on longitudinal testing in preclinical AD.
        Alzheimer's & Dementia. 2020; 16: 552-560
        • Bettcher BM
        • Johnson SC
        • Fitch R
        • et al.
        Cerebrospinal fluid and plasma levels of inflammation differentially relate to CNS markers of Alzheimer's disease pathology and neuronal damage.
        J Alzheimer's Dis. 2018; 62: 385-397
        • Michaud M
        • Balardy L
        • Moulis G
        • et al.
        Proinflammatory cytokines, aging, and age-related diseases.
        J Am Med Dir Assoc. 2013; 14: 877-882
        • Villar-Fincheira P
        • Sanhueza-Olivares F
        • Norambuena-Soto I
        • et al.
        Role of interleukin-6 in vascular health and disease.
        Front Mole Biosci. 2021; 8: 79
        • Zuliani G
        • Ranzini M
        • Guerra G
        • et al.
        Plasma cytokines profile in older subjects with late onset Alzheimer's disease or vascular dementia.
        J Psychiatr Res. 2007; 41: 686-693
        • Leening MJ
        • Ferket BS
        • Steyerberg EW
        • et al.
        Sex differences in lifetime risk and first manifestation of cardiovascular disease: prospective population based cohort study.
        Bmj. 2014; : 349
        • Fornage M
        • Chiang YA
        • O'Meara ES
        • et al.
        Biomarkers of inflammation and MRI-Defined small vessel disease of the brain: the Cardiovascular Health Study.
        Stroke. 2008; 39: 1952-1959
        • Bettcher BM
        • Mungas D
        • Patel N
        • et al.
        Neuroanatomical substrates of executive functions: beyond prefrontal structures.
        Neuropsychologia. 2016; 85: 100-109
        • Kramer J
        • Reed BR
        • Mungas D
        • et al.
        Executive dysfunction in subcortical ischaemic vascular disease.
        J Neurol, Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2002; 72: 217-220
        • Spencer JL
        • Waters EM
        • Romeo RD
        • et al.
        Uncovering the mechanisms of estrogen effects on hippocampal function.
        Front Neuroendocrinol. 2008; 29: 219-237
        • Kato A
        • Hojo Y
        • Higo S
        • et al.
        Female hippocampal estrogens have a significant correlation with cyclic fluctuation of hippocampal spines.
        Front Neural Circuits. 2013; 7: 149
        • Cui J
        • Shen Y
        • Li R
        Estrogen synthesis and signaling pathways during aging: from periphery to brain.
        Trends Mol Med. 2013; 19: 197-209
        • Aryan L
        • Younessi D
        • Zargari M
        • et al.
        The role of estrogen receptors in cardiovascular disease.
        Int J Mol Sci. 2020; 21
        • Zhao D
        • Guallar E
        • Ouyang P
        • et al.
        Endogenous sex hormones and incident cardiovascular disease in post-menopausal women.
        J Am Coll Cardiol. 2018; 71: 2555-2566

      Linked Article